Skip to main content

Are surgeons and anesthesiologists lying to each other or gaming the system? A national random sample survey about “truth-telling practices” in the perioperative setting in the United States

Abstract

Background

The perioperative setting demands strong teamwork to ensure safe patient care, but anecdotally surgeons and anesthesiologists are not always fully truthful with each other. The present study sought to determine the frequency of misrepresentation of the truth in the perioperative setting.

Methods

Direct mailed survey in the United States about misrepresenting information to colleagues in a national random sample of 1130 anesthesiologists and 1130 surgeons.

Results

Reflecting the sensitive nature of these questions, only 252 (11 %) surveys were returned-128/1130 by anesthesiologists and 124/1130 by surgeons. While modest numbers of both anesthesiologists (34/128, 27 %) and surgeons (8/124, 7 %) acknowledged misreporting information at least once per month, misreporting was considerably more common among responding anesthesiologists. Among anesthesiologists the majority (68 %) were concerned that surgeons misreported information to them once a month or more often, though only 8 % of surgeons shared reciprocal concerns. More than a third of responding anesthesiologists (36 %) reported having seen their teachers misreport information to surgeons during their training.

Conclusions

These findings, though preliminary due to the small sample, raise concerns about a possible culture of misrepresentation, passed on between generations, in some perioperative environments. Misreporting of information should be examined in more detail and addressed at local levels whenever it is found. Further research is required to determine if the reported behaviors represent routine gaming of perioperative care systems or deliberate and intentional deception. Strategies aimed at fostering conditions in which open honest communication can thrive should be investigated.

Background

There is increasing attention to safety culture, teamwork and communication practices in medicine [1, 2]. This focus has lead to large initiatives focused on effective communication skills [3]. As a result of such initiatives, the contribution of dysfunctional behavior in the perioperative setting is increasingly being scrutinized as a potential source of adverse outcomes and healthcare worker job dissatisfaction [4, 5]. Among these dysfunctional behaviors may be intentional physician-to-physician deception.

The phenomenon of intentional misrepresentation of the truth among physicians is little studied. Physicians occasionally misrepresent the truth to insurance companies to gain coverage for their patients [68], and there is a modest literature on the evolution of physician attitudes towards misrepresentation to patients with poor prognoses [9]. But almost nothing is known about whether physicians ever intentionally deceive each other in the course of patient care.

Anecdotally, anesthesiologists and surgeons may misrepresent the truth to each other in the course of perioperative care, such as by misreporting blood losses, the urgency of a case, or the volume of fluids given. Research has shown that surgeons are often inaccurate in their prediction of the duration of cases [1012], but it is unknown whether this inaccuracy is intentional or not. Similarly, documentation in the anesthetic record can vary considerably when record keeping is automated as compared to manual documentation by an anesthesia provider [13, 14], but the reasons for these discrepancies and whether they are intentional or not are also unknown.

Methods

To test the hypothesis that some degree of intentional misrepresentation might occur among physicians in the perioperative setting we conducted a pilot survey of anesthesiologists and surgeons nationally.

After obtaining approval from the Partners Human Research CommitteeFootnote 1 we mailed a self-administered survey to 1130 anesthesiologists and 1130 surgeons drawn from the American Medical Association (AMA) Masterfile, which includes all physicians practicing in the United States. After removing ineligible recipients, the sample of anesthesiologists was 1084 and for surgeons it was 1090. As an incentive, respondents were entered into a raffle to win an iPod. Six months after the initial mailing a follow-up post-card was mailed requesting that the original survey be returned or directing respondents to a secure website to enter responses.

To assess the frequency of misreporting information among anesthesiologists and surgeons, each survey included a set of potential facts (such as the amount of blood loss, the urgency of the case, or minor complications not apparent to the other physician) that could be misreported.  Each list was tailored to anesthesia or surgical practice (complete lists are available from the authors).  Survey recipients were asked to report how often, if ever, they had “knowingly mis-reported (or chosen not to report) the following intra-operative information to any member of the surgical/anesthesia team?” Responses on a 6-point likert scale ranged from 1) daily, 2) once or twice a week, 3) once or twice a month, 4) once or twice a year, 5) less than once a year, 6) and never.

Next, a list of potential justifications for misreporting was presented and respondents were asked “How important are the following possible justifications for deciding to mis-report (or not report) information to the surgical/anesthesia team.” Responses on a 5-point likert scale ranged from 1) very unimportant, 2) somewhat unimportant, 3) somewhat important, 4) very important, and 5) not applicable or I never mis-report. Respondents were also asked “in the last year, how often have you been concerned that your surgical/anesthesia colleagues have misrepresented the truth to you?” with 6 response options ranging from”daily” to “never”.

Respondents were informed in the cover letter that their responses would remain strictly confidential and that the Partners Human Research Committee approved the study protocol. Because of the sensitive nature of the survey questions, each subject was randomly assigned a unique identifier. Mailings were prepared in Chicago but return-mailed to the Boston-based investigators. The key linking unique identifiers to subjects was kept in Chicago, thereby preventing the Boston based team from linking responses to subjects.

Survey results were tabulated and demographics of responders and non-responders were compared by cross-indexing the unique identifier to demographic data in the AMA Physician Masterfile.  Descriptive statistics were presented for demographics and for survey results. Continuous variables were summarized in means and 95% confidence intervals while categorical variables were reported as frequencies and proportions. Demographics and survey responses of responders vs. non-responders, and anesthesiologists vs. surgeons were compared using two-sample Student’s t-test or chi-square test. All tests were two sided with a significance level of 0.05.

Results and discussion

Given the highly sensitive nature of the questions we asked, it is not surprising that this pilot survey generated a relatively low response rate (11 %, 252/2260); 128/1084 by anesthesiologists and 124/1090 by surgeons. By the same token, we suspect our exploratory findings on the frequency of anesthesiologist-surgeon deception may be conservative, given the socially undesirable nature of admitting to intentional deception of colleagues. More importantly, despite being far from definitive, our results raise provocative questions about truth-telling practices among physicians in the perioperative setting.

There were no significant differences between respondents and non-respondents for age or years since graduation, although more male surgeons did not respond to the survey (P = 0.009) (Table 1). Amongst respondents, there were no significant differences between anesthesiologists and surgeons for age, sex, years in practice, hospital size, English as a first language, or self reported religious status (Table 2).

Table 1 Respondents vs non-respondents demographics from AMA Masterfile
Table 2 Respondent demographics–self reported

Among the 252 respondents to our survey, up to 27 % of anesthesiologists and 7 % of surgeons admitted to misrepresenting the truth to each other at least once a month regarding a number of factors that can influence perioperative care (Tables 3 and 4). The justifications reported typically involved concerns around the adverse consequences of being truthful; in particular, paternalistic concerns about disclosing truthful information to a colleague who might not understand or who would then demand unreasonable steps (Table 5). Such concerns were the drivers of intentional misrepresentation much more often than worries about personal blame, chastisement or legal consequences.

Table 3 Most commonly misreported events by Anesthesiologists (a)
Table 4 Most commonly misreported events by Surgeons (a)
Table 5 Justifications considered “Very Important” when misrepresentation was acknowledged (*)

When clinical decision making is based on erroneous information, a cascading series of events can occur, leading to harm to patients and adverse consequences for physicians. This is indicated by the fact that 7 % of responding anesthesiologists and 2 % of surgeons said that a patient had been harmed by their misrepresentation of the truth to a colleague (data not shown in tables). Alternatively adverse consequences may occur to the physician who is misrepresenting or receives misrepresented information. In this survey 1 % of anesthesiologists and 4 % of surgeons reported suffering adverse consequences themselves due to an episode of misrepresentation (data not shown in tables). The nature of harms to patients and consequences to physicians cannot be identified from the present survey and requires further investigation.

The greatest impact of dishonest communication between colleagues in the perioperative setting may be that it damages teamwork among professionals engaged in high-stakes activities.  Candid and open communication and shared decision-making are foundational to optimizing teamwork. Studies have shown correlations between such team skills and actual or potential improved outcomes for patients [15, 16].

What can be learned from these pilot data? First, the motivations for intentional misrepresentation of the truth to colleagues likely represents a spectrum, ranging from gaming of the perioperative care system to optimize the ability to accomplish what physicians feel to be in the best interests of their patient to willful misrepresentation and deception (or lying) with a view to obfuscating facts to obtain personal benefits. Our results suggest the former motivation predominates, though future research should aim to better address the issue of motivation.

Second, the apparent asymmetry of our survey results requires further investigation. Anesthesiologists were both more likely to report misrepresenting facts to surgeons and more likely to be concerned about surgeons misrepresenting facts to them (Table 6). Perhaps of greater concern, more anesthesiologists (36 %) than surgeons (8 %) reported that they had seen their teachers engage in misrepresentation of the truth during their training. It is possible that anesthesiologists are more willing to be honest about misrepresenting issues to surgeons on a survey, or it could be that intentional misrepresentation really is more common within the anesthesia community. This question deserves focused attention, given its potential import for the profession, especially if in some cases deliberate misrepresentation is being taught, explicitly or implicitly, to trainees, perhaps as an anticipatory strategy for controlling or gaming the perioperative environment.

Table 6 How often have you been concerned your counterpart is misrepresenting information (in the past year)?

Third, we hope these preliminary results will spark more formal study of these issues. This poll was intended to generate conversation and hypotheses. After all, if there is a possibility that misrepresentation is becoming normative, on the assumption that one’s anesthesiology or surgeon counterpart is also engaging in a similar practice, this deserves direct and focused attention. In this regard, our results are especially troubling if our estimates for prevalence are low. In addition, while some might argue that minor misrepresentations comprise a normal social practice, our results also suggest that a potential for harm to patients and physicians exists and that further discussion, exploration and investigation is warranted.

Fourth, with regard to future research, this pilot work suggests that any future survey research on this topic will need to include robust efforts to obtain high response rates; and it is possible that the very sensitive nature of this issue will always preclude obtaining high levels of response. Other approaches to this area of enquiry might yield valuable insights, such as using qualitative data or the examination of automated data collected during surgery.

Finally, while our study focused on misrepresentations between surgeons and anesthesiologists, it is reasonable to be concerned that misrepresentation of medical practice might occur between other health professionals. For example, there are case reports of nursing staff intentionally hastening a patient’s death without explicit authority to do so and in the absence of formal documentation [17]. Residents have also reportedly engaged in sham resuscitations when they believed orders for resuscitation were not warranted [18]. These examples suggest that misrepresentation is probably more widespread in medicine than just between physician colleagues in the perioperative setting.

Notes

  1. The Partners Human Research Committee is the Institutional Review Board for Brigham and Women’s Hospital and Massachusetts General Hospital comprising amongst others, ethicists, physicians, and lay persons.

References

  1. Neily J, Mills PD, Young-Xu Y, Carney BT, West P, Berger DH, et al. Association between implementation of a medical team training program and surgical mortality. JAMA. 2010;304:1693–700.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Nurok M, Sundt TM, Frankel A. Teamwork and communication in the operating room: relationship to discrete outcomes and research challenges. Anesthesiol Clin. 2011;29:1–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. National Research Council (US) Committee on the Assessment of 21st Century Skills. Assessing 21st century skills: summary of a workshop. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US); 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Leape LL, Shore MF, Dienstag JL, Mayer RJ, Edgman-Levitan S, Meyer GS, et al. Perspective: a culture of respect, part 1: the nature and causes of disrespectful behavior by physicians. Acad Med. 2012;87:845–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Leape LL, Shore MF, Dienstag JL, Mayer RJ, Edgman-Levitan S, Meyer GS, et al. Perspective: a culture of respect, part 2: creating a culture of respect. Acad Med. 2012;87:853–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Freeman VG, Rathore SS, Weinfurt KP, Schulman KA, Sulmasy DP. Lying for patients: physician deception of third-party payers. Arch Intern Med. 1999;159:2263–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Wynia MK, Cummins DS, VanGeest JB, Wilson IB. Physician manipulation of reimbursement rules for patients: between a rock and a hard place. JAMA. 2000;283:1858–65.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Werner RM, Alexander GC, Fagerlin A, Ubel PA. Lying to insurance companies: the desire to deceive among physicians and the public. Am J Bioeth. 2004;4:53–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Novack DH, Plumer R, Smith RL, Ochitill H, Morrow GR, Bennett JM. Changes in physicians’ attitudes toward telling the cancer patient. JAMA. 1979;241:897–900.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Wright IH, Kooperberg C, Bonar BA, Bashein G. Statistical modeling to predict elective surgery time. Comparison with a computer scheduling system and surgeon-provided estimates. Anesthesiology. 1996;85:1235–45.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Strum DP, Sampson AR, May JH, Vargas LG. Surgeon and type of anesthesia predict variability in surgical procedure times. Anesthesiology. 2000;92:1454–66.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Eijkemans MJC, van Houdenhoven M, Nguyen T, Boersma E, Steyerberg EW, Kazemier G. Predicting the unpredictable: a new prediction model for operating room times using individual characteristics and the surgeon’s estimate. Anesthesiology. 2010;112:41–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. van Schalkwyk JM, Lowes D, Frampton C, Merry AF. Does manual anaesthetic record capture remove clinically important data? Br J Anaesth. 2011;107:546–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Wauben LSGL, van Grevenstein WMU, Goossens RHM, van der Meulen FH, Lange JF. Operative notes do not reflect reality in laparoscopic cholecystectomy. Br J Surg. 2011;98:1431–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Mazzocco K, Petitti DB, Fong KT, Bonacum D, Brookey J, Graham S, et al. Surgical team behaviors and patient outcomes. Am J Surg. 2009;197:678–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Haynes AB, Weiser TG, Berry WR, Lipsitz SR, Breizat A-HS, Dellinger EP, et al. Changes in safety attitude and relationship to decreased postoperative morbidity and mortality following implementation of a checklist-based surgical safety intervention. BMJ Qual Saf. 2011;20:102–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Asch DA. The role of critical care nurses in euthanasia and assisted suicide. N Engl J Med. 1996;334:1374–9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Muller JH. Shades of blue: the negotiation of limited codes by medical residents. Soc Sci Med. 1992;34:885–98.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

No Funding by the National Institutes of Health (NIH), Howard Hughes Medical Institute (HHMI), Medical Research Council (MRC), and/or Wellcome Trust.

Dr. Yan Ma’s research is supported by grants from Clinical Translational Science Center (NIH UL1-RR024996) and Agency for Healthcare Research & Quality (AHRQ R01HS021734).

IRB/Ethics Board

Partners Human Research Committee

116 Huntington Ave, Boston, MA 02116, USA; Tel: +1- 617-424-4100, Fax: +1-617-424-4199.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael Nurok.

Additional information

Competing interests

None of the authors have any financial or non-financial competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

MN, MW, SS participated in the study design, conduct of the study, data collection, data analysis, and manuscript preparation. LL, YM participated in data analysis and manuscript preparation. AK participated in the conduct of the study and data collection. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nurok, M., Lee, Yy., Ma, Y. et al. Are surgeons and anesthesiologists lying to each other or gaming the system? A national random sample survey about “truth-telling practices” in the perioperative setting in the United States. Patient Saf Surg 9, 34 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13037-015-0080-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13037-015-0080-7

Keywords